Does White Pride Lead to Prejudice?

By Rodolfo Mendoza-Denton | April 26, 2012 | 0 comments

Does identifying as "White" predict negative attitudes towards diversity? That depends on how conscious you are of power, says new research.

In recent years, research on White identity has gained traction in the psychological literature.

Researchers and clinicians have grown to realize that this group struggles with questions such as, “What does it mean to be White in my culture?” and, “What does being White mean to me?”

One of the most interesting—and important—questions relevant to White identity has to do with its relationship to intergroup atttitudes. Research looking into this question is all over the map, with some findings suggesting a positive link (that is, greater identification leading to more positive intergroup attitudes), other findings suggesting a negative link, and still others finding suggesting no link at all.

When science stumbles on these kinds of inconsistent, “all over the map” relationships, it usually means that there is another factor playing a role that researchers have not yet accounted for.

For example, ethnic identity among minority students has sometimes been linked to better academic outcomes, and sometimes linked to worse academic outcomes. My own research (Mendoza-Denton, Pietrzak, and Downey, 2008) has found that whether students worry that they may be discriminated against in their schooling context determines whether their ethnic identity serves as a strength or a risk factor.

My colleagues Matt Goren and Victoria Plaut at UC Berkeley have tackled the confusing relationship between White identity and intergroup attitudes in a 2011 paper published in the journal Self and Identity.

Goren and Plaut recognized that White identity is not a unitary phenomenon; rather, that it can take different forms. The researchers discovered that although some Whites were indeed weakly identified, those who identified strongly fell into two primary groups.

One group very much identified with being White, but in a way that allowed them to recognize White privilege (see this interesting post by Mikhail Lyubansky, “Ten Things Everyone Should Know About White Privilege Today”). The researchers called this group the the “power cognizant” group. The other group also identified with being White, but described their experience much more in terms of how proud they were to be a part of the group. This group the researchers termed the “prideful” group.

Once the researchers divided the highly identified Whites into these two groups, the picture rapidly came into focus.

Even though these two groups had similar levels of White identification, the power-cognizant group held significantly more positive attitudes towards diversity, as measured with items such as, “Universities should foster environments where differences are valued,” and, “A university education should expose students to the important differences in ideas and vaues that exist in the world.” The prideful group, by contrast, did not endorse these items as strongly, and showed a greater tendency towards prejudice than the power-cognizant group.

The key here is that if one were not to distinguish between these two groups, the research would have found no relationship at all between White identification and attitudes towards diversity!

There are two take-home points.

The first is that our scientific conclusions are limited by the types of questions we ask, so that few, if any, scientific findings are really the last word on a given topic.

The second is more subtle, and extremely important for positive intergroup relations. Just because someone has a strong ethnic identity, we cannot make direct conclusions about their attitudes on other topics. This applies not just to Whites, but to members of other groups as well.

Tracker Pixel for Entry
 
 
 
About The Author

Rodolfo Mendoza-Denton is an associate professor of psychology at the University of California, Berkeley, and the co-editor of the Greater Good book, Are We Born Racist?: New Insights from Neuroscience and Positive Psychology. He is a former director of the Greater Good Science Center.

  

Like this article?

Here's what you can do:

Donate
 
  
 
blog comments powered by Disqus
 

Most...

  
  

Greater Good Events

The Greater Good Science Center Summer Institute for Educators 2016
Clark Kerr Campus, UC-Berkeley
Sunday, June 26-Friday, July 1, 2016 OR Friday, July 15-Wednesday July 20, 2016 OR Sunday, July 24-Friday, July 29, 2016


The Greater Good Science Center Summer Institute for Educators 2016

The GGSC’s six-day Summer Institute equips educators with social-emotional learning tools that benefit both students and teachers.


» ALL EVENTS
 
 

Take a Greater Good Quiz!

How compassionate are you? How generous, grateful, or forgiving? Find out!

» TAKE A QUIZ
 

Watch Greater Good Videos

Jon Kabat-Zinn

Talks by inspiring speakers like Jon Kabat-Zinn, Dacher Keltner, and Barbara Fredrickson.

Watch
 

Greater Good Resources

 
 
» MORE STUDIES
 
 
» MORE ORGS
 

Book of the Week

The Brain That Changes Itself By Norman Doidge Explores the significance of neuroplasticity, where thoughts and experience can change the shape of the brain over time, engaging the reader with stories of miraculous recovery.

» READ MORE
 
Is she flirting with you? Take the quiz and find out.
"Greater Good offers a first-rate service to those who want to track new and important research findings in social and emotional intelligence."  
Daniel Goleman

Best-selling author,
Emotional Intelligence and Social Intelligence
(www.danielgoleman.info)

thnx advertisement